Apophenia as divergent thinking: Role of apophenia in creativity
Abstract
This paper examines the relationship between apophenia—the tendency to see patterns where there are none (Brugger, 2001), and creativity—understood as the production of new and appropriate (useful) ideas, solutions, or products (Nijstad et al., 2010; Runco & Jaeger, 2012). To do so, we first outline the historical background of the study of creativity, the contemporary definition of creativity, and both traditional and contemporary models of creativity. These sections provide a theoretical framework for understanding creativity through both processes of divergent generation of new ideas and selective or convergent control processes which make those new ideas adaptive/appropriate. Precisely contemporary cognitivist models view creativity as non-specific, fundamental cognition and recognize both divergent and convergent processes as necessary for creative ideas (Cropley, 2006; Ward, Smith, & Finke, 1999), while traditional models understand creativity as specific cognition (Mednick, 1962). Apophenia, besides referring to false positives, also represents the production of new but useless and inappropriate ideas and is, hence, the ideal example that reveals the need for establishing the necessary role of convergent processes alongside divergent ones in creativity. Examining the relationship between apophenia and creativity not only betters the understanding of creativity and provides further arguments for contemporary models of creativity as non-specific cognition but also positions apophenia as an underinvestigated phenomenon in relation to cognitive processes of idea production. To examine this relationship, it is necessary to take the position of contemporary cognitivist models of creativity. Therefore, the formation of new and unusual ideas can be seen as a consequence of divergent processes exclusively. Convergent processes further limit the divergent ones and enable the formation of new and useful ideas that are rightfully called creative. Given the assumptions about the roles divergent and convergent processes have in creativity (Cropley, 2006; Ward, Smith, & Finke, 1999), apophenia can be seen as another result of divergent processes. In other words, if convergent processes do not mediate divergent ones, divergent thinking consequently leads to new and useless— apophenic—ideas.
References
Amabile, T. M., Goldfarb, P., & Brackfleld, S. C. (1990). Social influences on creativity: Evaluation, coaction, and surveillance. Creativity research journal, 3(1), 6–21. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419009534330
Barrett, J. L. (2000). Exploring the natural foundations of religion. Trends in cognitive sciences, 4(1), 29–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01419-9
Barron, F. (1955). The disposition toward originality. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 51(3), 478. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048073
Becker, M. (1995). Nineteenth-century foundations of creativity research. Creativity Research Journal, 8(3), 219–229. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj0803_2
Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Revisiting Mednick’s model on creativity‐related differences in associative hierarchies. Evidence for a common path to uncommon thought. The Journal of creative behavior, 47(4), 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.35
Blain, S. D., Longenecker, J. M., Grazioplene, R. G., Klimes-Dougan, B., & DeYoung, C. G. (2020). Apophenia as the disposition to false positives: A unifying framework for openness and psychoticism. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(3), 279. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000504
Brugger, P. (2001). From haunted brain to haunted science: A cognitive neuroscience view of paranormal and pseudoscientific thought. In J. Houran, & R. Lange (Eds.), Hauntings and poltergeists: Multidisciplinary perspectives (pp. 195–213). McFarland.
Brugger, P., & Graves, R. E. (1997). Testing vs. believing hypotheses: Magical ideation in the judgement of contingencies. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2(4), 251–272. https://doi.org/10.1080/135468097396270
Carpenter, P. A., Just, M. A., & Shell, P. (1990). What one intelligence test measures: a theoretical account of the processing in the Raven Progressive Matrices Test. Psychological review, 97(3), 404. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.404
Cattell, R. B. (1943). The measurement of adult intelligence. Psychological bulletin, 40(3), 153. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0059973
Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent thinking. Creativity research journal, 18(3), 391–404. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1803_13
Dagnall, N., Denovan, A., Drinkwater, K., Parker, A., & Clough, P. (2016). Toward a better understanding of the relationship between belief in the paranormal and statistical bias: the potential role of schizotypy. Frontiers in psychology, 1045. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01045
DeYoung, C. G. (2015). Openness/intellect: A dimension of personality reflecting cognitive exploration. In M. Mikulincer, P. R. Shaver, M. L. Cooper, & R. J. Larsen (Eds.), APA handbook of personality and social psychology, Vol. 4. Personality processes and individual differences (pp. 369–399). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/14343-017
DeYoung, C. G., Grazioplene, R. G., & Peterson, J. B. (2012). From madness to genius: The Openness/Intellect trait domain as a paradoxical simplex. Journal of Research in Personality, 46(1), 63–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2011.12.003
Diana, L., Frei, M., Chesham, A., de Jong, D., Chiffi, K., Nyffeler, T., ... & Müri, R. M. (2021). A divergent approach to pareidolias—Exploring creativity in a novel way. Psychology of aesthetics, creativity, and the arts, 15(2), 313. https://doi.org/10.1037/aca0000293
Epley, N., Waytz, A., Akalis, S., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). When we need a human: Motivational determinants of anthropomorphism. Social cognition, 26(2), 143–155. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2008.26.2.143
Feist, G. J. (2010). The function of personality in creativity: The nature and nurture of the creative personality. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 113–130). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511763205.009
Feist, G. J. (2019). The function of personality in creativity: Updates on the creative personality. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 353–373). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316979839.019
Fyfe, S., Williams, C., Mason, O. J., & Pickup, G. J. (2008). Apophenia, theory of mind and schizotypy: perceiving meaning and intentionality in randomness. Cortex, 44(10), 1316–1325. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2007.07.009
Gilhooly, K. J., & Murphy, P. (2005). Differentiating insight from non-insight problems. Thinking & Reasoning, 11(3), 279–302. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546780442000187
Grant, P., Balser, M., Munk, A. J. L., Linder, J., & Hennig, J. (2014). A false-positive detection bias as a function of state and trait schizotypy in interaction with intelligence. Frontiers in psychiatry, 5, 135. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00135
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0063487
Johnson, D. D. (2009). The error of God: Error management theory, religion, and the evolution of cooperation. In S. Levin (Ed.), Games, groups, and the global good (pp. 169–180). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-540-85436-4_10
Johnson, D. D., Blumstein, D. T., Fowler, J. H., & Haselton, M. G. (2013). The evolution of error: Error management, cognitive constraints, and adaptive decision-making biases. Trends in ecology & evolution, 28(8), 474–481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2013.05.014
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Jiménez, L., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2010). Implicit learning as an ability. Cognition, 116(3), 321–340. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.011
Kozbelt, A., Beghetto, R. A., & Runco, M. A. (2010). Theories of creativity. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 20–47). Cambridge University Press.
Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological review, 69(3), 220. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0048850
Mishara, A. L. (2010). Klaus Conrad (1905–1961): Delusional mood, psychosis, and beginning schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 36(1), 9–13. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp144
Neyman, J., & Pearson, E. S. (1933). IX. On the problem of the most efficient tests of statistical hypotheses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A, Containing Papers of a Mathematical or Physical Character, 231(694–706), 289–337. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsta.1933.0009
Nijstad, B. A., De Dreu, C. K., Rietzschel, E. F., & Baas, M. (2010). The dual pathway to creativity model: Creative ideation as a function of flexibility and persistence. European review of social psychology, 21(1), 34–77. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463281003765323
Nouri, R., Erez, M., Rockstuhl, T., Ang, S., Leshem‐Calif, L., & Rafaeli, A. (2013). Taking the bite out of culture: The impact of task structure and task type on overcoming impediments to cross‐cultural team performance. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 34(6), 739–763. https://doi.org/10.1002/job.1871
Rietzschel, E. F., Slijkhuis, J. M., & Van Yperen, N. W. (2014). Task structure, need for structure, and creativity. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(4), 386–399. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2024
Rominger, C., Weiss, E. M., Fink, A., Schulter, G., & Papousek, I. (2011). Allusive thinking (cognitive looseness) and the propensity to perceive “meaningful” coincidences. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(8), 1002–1006. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.012
Runco, M. A. (2004). Creativity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 657–687. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141502
Runco, M. A., & Acar, S. (2019). Divergent thinking. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 224–254). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316979839.013
Runco, M. A., & Albert, R. S. (2010). Creativity research: A historical view. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 3–19). Cambridge University Press.
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity research journal, 24(1), 92–96. https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2012.650092
Ruscio, A. M., & Amabile, T. M. (1999). Effects of instructional style on problem-solving creativity. Creativity research journal, 12(4), 251–266. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326934crj1204_3
Stein, M. I. (1953). Creativity and culture. The journal of psychology, 36(2), 311–322. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.1953.9712897
Sternberg, R. J., & Kaufman, J. C. (2010). Constraints on creativity. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 467–482). Cambridge University Press.
Van Elk, M., Rutjens, B. T., van der Pligt, J., & Van Harreveld, F. (2016). Priming of supernatural agent concepts and agency detection. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 6(1), 4–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2014.933444
Van Prooijen, J. W., Douglas, K. M., & De Inocencio, C. (2018). Connecting the dots: Illusory pattern perception predicts belief in conspiracies and the supernatural. European journal of social psychology, 48(3), 320–335. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2331
Wang, J. J., & Kaufman, A. S. (1993). Changes in fluid and crystallized intelligence across the 20-to 90-year age range on the K-BIT. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 11(1), 29–37. https://doi.org/10.1177/073428299301100104
Ward, T. B., & Kolomyts, Y. (2010). Cognition and creativity. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 93–112). Cambridge University Press.
Ward, T. B., Smith, S. M., & Finke, R. A. (1999). Creative cognition. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 189–212). Cambridge University Press.
Willard, A. K., & Norenzayan, A. (2013). Cognitive biases explain religious belief, paranormal belief, and belief in life’s purpose. Cognition, 129(2), 379–391. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.016
Wiseman, R., Watt, C., Gilhooly, K., & Georgiou, G. (2011). Creativity and ease of ambiguous figural reversal. British Journal of Psychology, 102(3), 615–622. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02031.x
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License.